Is Freedom Compatible with Moral Neuroenhancement?
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.18662/brain/11.2Sup1/96Keywords:
Moral neuroenhancement, freedom, moral bioenhancement, oxytocin, serotonin, tCDS, lithium, Omega-3, freedom to fall, mental freedom, empathy, moral responsibility, moral sensitivity, moral motivation, counter-moral emotions, racial bias, aggressionAbstract
New discoveries in the fields of genetics and neurobiology from the last decades have offered scientists more understanding regarding the way in which our biology can influence our behavior and have made more appealing the idea of moral neuroenhancement (or bioenhancement), but one of the main objections to this way of enhancing moral motivation is a hypothetical threat to freedom. Critics argue that our freedom would be in jeopardy if we allowed interventions at our genetical level to predispose us to a certain kind of behaviour, even if that behaviour would be moral. My paper wishes to explore the relation between freedom and moral neuroenhancement in order to see if there really is an incompatibility between these 2 concepts, or if there’s a way in which freedom itself could be enhanced, so that the individual could have more options at his disposal. If moral neuroenhancement can lead to a moral life by improving moral character and predisposing the individual towards being more virtuous, while also leaving our freedom and autonomy unaltered, then we might have a strong reason to consider that freedom can be compatible with moral neuroenhancement and even enhanced by it. In this paper I will argue for the idea that not only is moral neuroenhancement compatible with freedom, but that it can actually expand it and provide us with more moral options to choose from.
References
Aristotle. (2000). Nicomachean Ethics, Roger Crisp (ed./trans.). Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511802058
Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The Essential Difference: Male and Female Brains and the Truth about Autism. Basic Books.
Bostrom, N. & Ord, T. (2006). The Reversal Test: Eliminating the Status Quo Bias in Applied Ethics. Ethics, 116, 656–679.
Brunner, H. G., Nelen, M., Breakefield, X. O., Ropers, H. H. & Van Oost, B. A. (1993). Abnormal Behaviour Associated with a Point Mutation in the Structural Gene for Monoamine Oxidase A. Science, 262(5133), 578-580.
Bublitz, C. (2016). Moral enhancement and mental freedom. J ournal of Applied Philosophy, 33(1), 88–106.
Buchanan, A., Brock, D. W., Norman, D., & Winkler, D. (2000). From Chance to Choice: Genetics and Justice. Cambridge University Press.
Choy, O., Raine. A., & Hamilton, R. H. (2018). Stimulation of the prefrontal cortex reduces intentions to commit aggression: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, stratified, parallel-group trial. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(29), 6505–6512. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3317-17.2018
Conan, G. M. (2020). Frequently overlooked realistic moral Bioenhancement interventions. Journal of Medical Ethics, 46(1), 43–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/medethics-2019-105534
Crockett, M. J. (2014). Moral bioenhancement: a neuroscientific perspective. Journal of Medical Ethics 40(6), 370–371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/medethics-2012-101096
Crockett, M., Clark, L., Hauser, M., & Robbins, T. (2010). Serotonin selectively influences moral judgment and behavior through effects on harm aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 17433 - 17438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1009396107
De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, L. L., Handgraaf, M. J. J. Shalvi, S., Gerben A. Van Kleef, G. A., Baas, M., Femke S. Ten Velden, F. S., Van Dijk, E., & Feith, S. W. W. (2010). Neuropeptide Oxytocin Regulates Parochial Altruism in Intergroup Conflicts among Humans. Science, 328, 1408–1411. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1189047
De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, l. L., Van Kleef, G. A., Shalvi, S., & Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Oxytocin Promotes Human Ethnocentrism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1262–1266. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015316108
DeGrazia, D. (2014). Moral enhancement, freedom, and what we (should) value in moral behavior. Journal of Medical Ethics, 40, 361–368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/medethics-2012-101157
Dieguez, A., & Veliz, C. (2017). Would Moral Enhancement Limit Freedom? Topoi 38, 29-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11245-017-9466-8
Douglas, T. (2008). Moral enhancement. Journal of Applied Philosophy, 25(3), 228–245. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5930.2008.00412.x
Earp, B. D., Douglas, T., & Savulescu, J. (2017). Moral neuroenhancement. In S. Johnson and K. Rommelfanger (Eds.), Routledge Handbook of Neuroethics. Routledge. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK493126/
Gajos, J. M., & Beaver, K. M. (2016). The effect of omega-3 fatty acids on aggression: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 147–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.017
Goldstein, M. R., & Mascitelli, L. (2016). Is violence in part a lithium deficiency state? Medical Hypotheses, 89, 40–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2016.02.00
Harris, J. (2011). Moral enhancement and freedom. Bioethics, 25(2), 102–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8519.2010.01854.x
Harris, J. (2013). Moral progress and moral enhancement. Bioethics, 27(5), 285–290. https://doi.org/10.1111/bioe.12592
Harris, J. (2014). Taking liberties with free fall. Journal of Medical Ethics, 40(6), 371–374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/medethics-2012-101092
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin Increases Trust in Humans. Nature, 435(2), 673–676. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03701
Lazari-Radek, Katarzyna De, Singer, P. (2014). The Point of View of the Universe: Sidgwick and Contemporary Ethics. Oxford Scholarship Online: June 2014. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199603695.001.0001
Levy, N., Douglas, T., Kahane, G., Terbeck, S., Cowen, P. J., Hewstone, M., & Savulescu, J. (2014). Are you morally modified? The moral effects of widely used pharmaceuticals. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 21(2), 111-125. https://doi.org/10.1353/ppp.2014.0023
Milton, J. (2000) [1667]. Paradise Lost. In J. Leonard, (Ed.), Paradise Lost. Penguin Books.
Morell, V. (1993). Evidence Found for a Possible Aggression Gene. Science, 260, 1722-1723.
Müller-Oerlinghausen, B., & Lewitzka, U. (2010). Lithium reduces pathological aggression and suicidality: a mini-review. Neuropsychobiology, 62(1), 43–49.
Pachet, A. K., & Wisniewski, A. M. (2003). The effects of lithium on cognition: an updated review. Psychopharmacology, 170(3), 225–234. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-003-1592-x
Persson, I., & Savulescu, J. (2012). Unfit for the Future: The Need for Moral Enhancement, Oxford Scholarship Online, September 2012. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199653645.003.0010 .
President’s Council on Bioethics. (2003). Beyond Therapy: Biotechnology and the Pursuit of Happiness. Regan Books.
Raine, A. (2014). The anatomy of violence: the biological roots of crime. Penguin Books.
Sellaro, R., Derks, B., Nitsche, M. A., Hommel, B., Van den Wildenbergde, W. P. M., Van Dam, K., & Colzato, L. S. (2015). Reducing prejudice through brain stimulation. Brain Stimulation, 8(5), 891–897. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2015.04.003
Terao, T. (2008). Aggression, suicide, and lithium treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(10), 1356–1357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08040565
Tse, W. S. & Bond, A. J. (2002). Serotonergic Intervention Affects Both Social Dominance and Affiliative Behaviour, Psychopharmacology, 161, 324–330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-002-1049-7
Tuchman, B. W. (1978). A distant mirror: The calamitous 14th century. Knopf.
Zak, P. J. (2012). The Moral Molecule: The Source of Love and Prosperity. Dutton.
Zak, P., Kurzban, R., & Matzner, W. (2004). The Neurobiology of Trust. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032, 224–227. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1314.025
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2020 The Authors & LUMEN Publishing House

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
- Authors retain copyright and grant this journal right of first publication, with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work, with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g. post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g. in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as an earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
BRAIN. Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience Journal has an Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs
CC BY-NC-ND