Conspiracy Belief and Behavior in the COVID-19 Pandemic

How Belief in Conspiracy Theory Relates to Adherence to Quarantine Restrictions (Wearing Protective Equipment, Isolation, Hygiene) and Influences Antisocial Behavior such as Aggression and Selfishness, as well as Prosocial Behavior such as Help and Altruism

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.18662/brain/12.2/200

Keywords:

Belief in conspiracy, quarantine behavior, antisocial behavior, pro-social behavior

Abstract

This research was aimed at investigating the relationship between belief in conspiracy theory and personality behaviors in quarantine. Belief in conspiracy theory has been hypothesized to predict the degree of adherence to quarantine restrictions (wearing protective equipment, isolation, hygiene) and influences antisocial behavior such as aggression and selfishness, as well as prosocial behavior such as help and altruism. The obtained data of the empirical research indicate the absence of functional connections that could demonstrate the defining role of conspiracy mindset in the manifestations of prosocial and asocial behavior; however, there is still a statistical consistency of changes in indicators.

Also, preventive behavior was practically not associated with the personality's conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 (the regression equation explains less than 1% of the variance). It is emphasized that the data obtained contradict the results of some previous researches, which are devoted to the research of the behavioral consequences of conspiracy mindset and adherence to preventive measures in a pandemic, in particular.

Author Biographies

Nina Yarosh, V. N. Karazin Kharkiv National University

PhD, an associate professor of the Applied Psychology Department

Vladyslava Artiukhova, I. Kozhedub Air Force National University, Kharkiv, Ukraine

PhD,an associate professor of the Psychology and Pedagogy Department 

Oleksii Zimovin, V. N. Karazin Kharkiv National University

PhD,an associate professor of the Applied Psychology Department

References

Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20(3), 637–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160

Alper, S., Bayrak, F., & Yilmaz, O. (2020). Psychological correlates of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and preventive measures: Evidence from Turkey. Current Psychology 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00903-0

Banai, I. P., Banai, B., & Mikloušić, I. (2020). Beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories predict lower level of compliance with the preventive measures both directly and indirectly by lowering trust in government medical officials. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/yevq7

Bilewicz, M., Winiewski, M., Kofta, M., & Wójcik, A. (2013). Harmful ideas, the structure and consequences of anti-Semitic beliefs in Poland. Political Psychology, 34(6), 821–839. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12024

Bogart, L. M., & Thorburn, S. (2005). Are HIV/AIDS Conspiracy Beliefs a Barrier to HIV Prevention Among African Americans? Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 38(2), 213–218. https://doi.org/10.1097/00126334-200502010-00014

Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., & Golec de Zavala, A. (2016). Does self-love or self-hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Narcissism, self-esteem, and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 157–166. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615616170

Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(8), 1289–1293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027

Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017). The Psychology of Conspiracy Theories. Current directions in psychological science, 26(6), 538–542. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417718261

Einstein, K. L. & Glick, D. M. (2015). Do I think BLS data are BS? The consequences of conspiracy theories. Political Behavior, 37(3), 679–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-014-9287-z

Fetiskin, N., Kozlov, V., Manuilov, G. (2002). Diagnostika lichnostnoy ustanovki “al'truizm – egoizm” [Diagnostics of the personal attitude “altruism - egoism”]. Socio-psychological diagnostics of the development of personality and small groups. Moscow: Izd-vo Instituta Psikhoterapii. 23–24. [in Russian]

Franks, B., Bangerter, A., Bauer, M.W., Hall, M. & Noort, M.C. (2017). Beyond “monologicality”? Exploring conspiracist worldviews. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 861. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00861

Gaston, G. B & Alleyne-Green, B. (2013). The impact of African Americans' beliefs about HIV medical care on treatment adherence: a systematic review and recommendations for interventions. AIDS Behav 17(1), 31–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10461-012-0323-x

Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15(4), 731–742. https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630

Golec de Zavala, A., Cichocka, A., & Iskra-Golec, I. (2013). Collective narcissism moderates the effect of in-group image threat on intergroup hostility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(6), 1019–1039. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032215

Goreis, A., & Voracek, M. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological research on conspiracy beliefs: Field characteristics, measurement instruments, and associations with personality traits. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 205. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00205

Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2015). Are the high authoritarians more prone to adopt conspiracy theories? The role of right-wing authoritarianism in conspiratorial thinking. In M. Bilewicz, A. Cichocka, & W. Soral (Eds.), The psychology of conspiracy (p. 99–121). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2018). How paranoid are conspiracy believers? Toward a more fine-grained understanding of the connect and disconnect between paranoia and belief in conspiracy theories. European journal of social psychology, 48(7), 909–926. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2494

Imhoff, R., Lamberty, P., Klein, O. (2018). Using power as a negative cue: How conspiracy mentality affects epistemic trust in sources of historical knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 1364–1379. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218768779

Imhoff, R., Dieterle, L., Lamberty, P. (2020). Resolving the puzzle of conspiracy worldview and political activism: Belief in secret plots decreases normative but increases nonnormative political engagement. Social Psychological and Personality Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619896491.

Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M. (2014). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 35–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018

Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., Leite, A. C., Schrader, T. (2019). Belief in conspiracy theories and intentions to engage in everyday crime. British Journal of Social Psychology 58(3), 534-549 https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12311

Khvan, A. A., Zaitsev, Yu.A., Kuznetsova, Yu. A. (2008). Opyt standartizatsii oprosnika izmereniya agressivnykh i vrazhdebnykh reaktsiy A. Bassa i A. Darki [Experience of standardization of the questionnaire for measuring aggressive and hostile reactions A. Bass and A. Darki]. Psychological diagnostics, 1, 54–58.

Klofstad, C.A., Uscinski, J.E., Connolly, J.M. et al. (2019). What drives people to believe in Zika conspiracy theories? Palgrave Commun, 5, 36. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0243-8

Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Vaughan, S. (2013). The pivotal role of perceived scientific consensus in acceptance of science. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 399–404. https://doi.org/10.1038/NCLIMATE1720

Marinthe, G., Brown, G., Delouvée, S. & Jolley, D. (2020). Looking out for myself: Exploring the relationship between conspiracy mentality, perceived personal risk, and COVID-19 prevention measures. Health Psychology, 25(4), 957–980. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12449

Nartova-Bochaver, S.K. (1992). Eksperimental'noye issledovaniye situatsionnoy izmenchivosti motivatsii pomoshchi [An experimental study of the situational variability of motivation for help]. Psychological Journal, 13 (4), 15-23.

Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy Theories and the Paranoid Style(s) of Mass Opinion. American journal of political science, 58 (4), 952–966. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12084

Parsons, S., Simmons, W., Shinhoster, F., & Kilburn, J. (1999). A test of the grapevine: An empirical examination of conspiracy theories among African Americans. Sociological Spectrum, 19(2), 201–222. https://doi.org/10.1080/027321799280235

Plohl, N., & Musil, B. (2020). Modeling compliance with covid-19 prevention guidelines: The critical role of trust in science. Psychology, Health & Medicine. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2020.1772988

Swami, V. (2012). Social psychological origins of conspiracy theories: The case of the Jewish conspiracy theory in Malaysia. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00280

Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133, 572–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006

Twenge, J., Baumeister, R., DeWall C. N., Ciarocco, N. J. & Bartels J.M. (2007). Social Exclusion Decreases Prosocial Behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92(1), 56–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.56

Uscinski, J. E., Parent, J. M. (2014). American Conspiracy Theories. New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199351800.001.0001

Van der Linden, SL. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Individual Differences 87, 171–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.045

Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(1), 109–115. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1922

Van Prooijen, J.-W. (2016). Sometimes inclusion breeds suspicion: Self-uncertainty and belongingness predict belief in conspiracy theories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 267–279. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2157

Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Douglas, K. M. (2018). Belief in conspiracy theories: Basic principles of an emerging research domain. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(7), 897–908. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2530

Van Prooijen, J. -W. Bilewicz, M., Witkowska, M., Pantazi, M., Gkinopoulos, Th. & Klein, O. (2019). Traumatic Rift: How Conspiracy Beliefs Undermine Cohesion After Societal Trauma? European Journal of Psychology, 15(1), 82–93. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699

Watson, P. J. & Morris, R. J. (1991). Narcissism, empathy and social desirability. Personality and Individual Differences, 12(6), 575–579. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(91)90253-8

Whitson, J. A. & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking Control Increases Illusory Pattern Perception. Science 322(5898):115–7. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845

Downloads

Published

2021-07-19

How to Cite

Yarosh, N., Artiukhova, V. ., & Zimovin, O. . (2021). Conspiracy Belief and Behavior in the COVID-19 Pandemic: How Belief in Conspiracy Theory Relates to Adherence to Quarantine Restrictions (Wearing Protective Equipment, Isolation, Hygiene) and Influences Antisocial Behavior such as Aggression and Selfishness, as well as Prosocial Behavior such as Help and Altruism. BRAIN. Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience, 12(2), 202-221. https://doi.org/10.18662/brain/12.2/200

Publish your work at the Scientific Publishing House LUMEN

It easy with us: publish now your work, novel, research, proceeding at Lumen Scientific Publishing House

Send your manuscript right now