Word Reading Accuracy as a Mediator for Fluency
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.18662/rrem/14.1/524Keywords:
nonword reading accuracy, word nonword reading fluency, rapid naming (RAN), word reading accuracy, word reading fluencyAbstract
This paper aims to test the predictive role of nonword reading abilities (accuracy and fluency) and rapid naming (RAN) over word reading fluency. Letter knowledge (LK) and phonological awareness (PA) for literacy were accounted for as control factors due to their ceiling effects. We test the direct impact of the main predictors and their indirect outcomes by controlling for word reading accuracy. To this end, 206 children participated and completed measures for nonword reading abilities, rapid naming, word reading accuracy, fluency, and letter knowledge, and phonological awareness. The results show that RAN and nonword reading accuracy are direct predictors of word reading accuracy. Word reading fluency is significantly associated with rapid naming. The effect of RAN, nonword reading accuracy, nonword reading fluency on word reading fluency is partially mediated by word reading accuracy. We discuss the practical implications.
References
Araújo, S., Pacheco, A., Faísca, L., Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2010). Visual rapid naming and phonological abilities: Different subtypes in dyslexic children. International Journal of Psychology, 45(6), 443-452. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2010.499949
Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of memory and language, 45(4), 751-774. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2785
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavský, M., Onochie-Quintanilla, E., Salas, N., Schoffelova, M., Defior, S., Mikulajova, M., Seidlová-Málková, G. & Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological science, 23(6), 678-686. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797611434536
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children. Language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 32(1), 38. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2001/004)
David, C., & Roșan, A. (2017). Repere diagnostice bazate pe dovezi științifice în tulburările de învățare [Scientific evidence-based diagnostic guidelines for learning disorders.]. Editura Argonaut & Limes. http://learnid.centre.ubbcluj.ro/resurse/2017_David_Rosan-Repere_diagnostice_in_TSI.pdf
David, C., Roşan, A, & Gavril, L. (2018). Reading Strategies of Romanian Readers with Dyslexia in Upper Primary Grades, Prima Educatione, 89-90. https://core.ac.uk/reader/235272600
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized' naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14(4), 471-479. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0
Dragomirescu, A. (2012). Ortografia limbii române: definiţie, scurt istoric, instrumente [Romanian language spelling: definition, short history, tools]. Revista Limba română, 1-2, 56-63. https://ibn.idsi.md/sites/default/files/imag_file/Ortografia%20limbii%20romane%2056_63.pdf
Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of research in reading, 18(2), 116-125. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/j.1467-9817.1995.tb00077.x
Ehri, L. C. (2014). Orthographic mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling memory, and vocabulary learning. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 5-21. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.819356
Eloranta, A. K., Närhi, V. M., Eklund, K. M., Ahonen, T. P., & Aro, T. I. (2019). Resolving reading disability—Childhood predictors and adult‐age outcomes. Dyslexia, 25(1), 20-37. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1605
Foulin, J. N. (2005). Why is letter-name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading and word reading accuracy 18(2), 129-155. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-004-5892-2
Fricke, S., Szczerbinski, M., Fox‐Boyer, A., & Stackhouse, J. (2016). Preschool predictors of early literacy acquisition in German‐speaking children. Reading research quarterly, 51(1), 29-53. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.116
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: a multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(1), 104. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.104
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word nonword reading and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 566. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566
Goswami, U. (2015). Sensory theories of developmental dyslexia: three challenges for research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(1), 43-54. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3836
Heggie, L., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2017). Reading longer words: Insights into multisyllabic word reading. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 2(1), 86-94. https://doi.org/10.1044/persp2.SIG1.86
Hudson, R. F., Lane, H. B., & Pullen, P. C. (2005). Reading fluency assessment and instruction: What, why, and how?. The Reading Teacher, 58(8), 702-714. https://doi.org/10.1598/RT.58.8.1
Katzir, T., Shaul, S., Breznitz, Z., & Wolf, M. (2004). The universal and the unique in dyslexia: A cross-linguistic investigation of reading and reading fluency in Hebrew-and English-speaking children with reading disorders. Reading and Word reading accuracy, 17(7-8), 739-768. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-004-2655-z
Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(3), 341-362. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.3.4
Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of educational psychology, 95(1), 3. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED438530.pdf
Landerl, K., Frith, U., & Wimmer, H. (1996). The intrusion of orthographic knowledge on phoneme awareness: Strong in normal readers, weak in dyslexic readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17(1), 1-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009437
Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children. Applied psycholinguistics, 21(2), 243-262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400002058
Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: an 8-year follow-up. Journal of educational psychology, 100(1), 150-161. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.150
Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H. H., Heene, M., De Jong, P. F., Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Parrila, R., & Georgiou, G. K. (2019). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23(3), 220-234. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1510936
Layes, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebaï, M. (2015). Reading Speed and Phonological Awareness Deficits Among Arabic‐Speaking Children with Dyslexia. Dyslexia, 21(1), 80-95. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1491
Lima, M., da Rosa Piccolo, L., Puntel Basso, F., Júlio-Costa, A., Lopes-Silva, J. B., Haase, V. G., & Salles, J. F. (2019). Neuropsychological and environmental predictors of reading performance in Brazilian children. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 9(3), 259-270. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2019.1575737
Logan, J. A., Schatschneider, C., & Wagner, R. K. (2011). Rapid serial naming and reading ability: The role of lexical access. Reading and Word reading accuracy, 24(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9199-1
Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and word reading accuracy, 15(7-8), 653-682. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020984704781
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0026744
Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430802631684
Moura, O., Moreno, J., Pereira, M., & Simões, M. R. (2015). Developmental dyslexia and phonological processing in European Portuguese orthography. Dyslexia, 21(1), 60-79. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1489
Mousty, P. & Leybaert, Jacqueline & Alegria, Jésus & Content, Alain & Morais, José. (1994). BELEC: Une batterie d'évaluation du langage écrit et de ses troubles. Evaluer les Troubles de la Lecture. 127-145. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/285880579_BELEC_Une_batterie_d'evaluation_du_langage_ecrit_et_de_ses_troubles/citation/download
Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual review of psychology, 63, 427-452. https://www.annualreviews.org/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431
Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G. C., & Georgiou, G. K. (2016). How is RAN related to reading fluency? A comprehensive examination of the prominent theoretical accounts. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 1217. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01217
Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Gottardo, A., & Geva, E. (2015). Cross-language transfer of word reading accuracy and word reading fluency in Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals: Script-universal and script-specific processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 96. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0036966
Poulsen, M., Juul, H., & Elbro, C. (2015). Multiple mediation analysis of the relationship between rapid naming and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 38(2), 124-140. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01547.x
Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppänen, P. H., Poikkeus, A. M., Tolvanen, A., Torppa, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2008). Developmental links of very early phonological and language skills to second-grade reading outcomes: Strong to accuracy and minor to fluency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(4), 353-370. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0022219407311747
Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (1995). Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva (Battery for assessing developmental dyslexia and disorthography). Edizioni O.S.
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Predicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: Contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ. Annals of Dyslexia, 48(1), 115-136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-998-0006-5
Schaars, M. M., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Word Decoding Development during Phonics Instruction in Children at Risk for Dyslexia. Dyslexia, 23(2), 141-160. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1556
Serrano, F., & Defior, S. (2008). Dyslexia speed problems in a transparent orthography. Annals of dyslexia, 58(1), 81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-008-0013-6
Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., Erskine, J. M., & Collaboration with COST Action A8 Network. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(2), 143-174. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2020). Annual Research Review: Reading disorders revisited–the critical importance of oral language. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13324
Stuart, M., & Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30(2), 139-181. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(88)90038-8
Sunde, K., Furnes, B., & Lundetræ, K. (2020). Does Introducing the Letters Faster Boost the Development of Children's Letter Knowledge, Word Reading, and Spelling in the First Year of School?. Scientific Studies of Reading, 24(2), 141-158. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1615491
Taub, G. E., & Szente, J. (2012). The impact of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness on the reading fluency of a minority student population. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 26(4), 359-370. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568543.2012.712084
Tilanus, E. A., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Diagnostic profiles of children with developmental dyslexia in a transparent orthography. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(11), 4194-4202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.039
Tong, X., McBride, C., Lo, J. C. M., & Shu, H. (2017). A three‐year longitudinal study of reading and spelling difficulty in Chinese developmental dyslexia: The matter of morphological awareness. Dyslexia, 23(4), 372-386. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1564
Toste, J. R., Williams, K. J., & Capin, P. (2017). Reading big words: Instructional practices to promote multisyllabic word reading fluency. Intervention in School and Clinic, 52(5), 270-278. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1053451216676797
Vaessen, A., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Faísca, L., Reis, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). The cognitive development of fluent word reading does not qualitatively differ between transparent and opaque orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 827. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0019465
Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51(1), 91-103. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90010-8
Wimmer, H., & Mayringer, H. (2002). Dysfluent reading in the absence of spelling difficulties: A specific disability in regular orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 272. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.272
Wimmer, H., & Schurz, M. (2010). Dyslexia in regular orthographies: manifestation and causation. Dyslexia, 16(4), 283-299. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.411
Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of learning disabilities, 33(4), 387-407. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F002221940003300409
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3. https://psycnet.apa.org/buy/2004-22408-001
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). The orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological science, 21(4), 551-559. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797610363406
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2022 The Authors & LUMEN Publishing House

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
- Authors retain copyright and grant this journalright of first publication, with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work, with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g. post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g. in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as an earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
Revista Romaneasca pentru Educatie Multidimensionala Journal has an Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs
CC BY-NC-ND